Horses have sex with humans

Live Cam Models - Online Now

People and some animals can and do have sexual exchanges. Religious people condemn this as being the most ungodly thing ever however we as humans. Ex-animal sanctuary worker had 1, pictures of humans having sex depicting sex acts between adults and dogs, cows, pigs and horses on. More Swiss people are having sex with horses, according to a sickening new report. There were cases of the maltreatment of horses in.

Some of them were exclusively into animals, while others had human partners, too. The most-preferred animal partners were dogs, but horses. Have You Ever Heard of The Man Who Died From Having Sex with a Horse? Zoo was also about the Enumclaw Horse Sex Case. . Given human and consensual sexual interaction, there was no reason for this law. kwansei.info › en_us › article › why-people-have-sex-with-animals.

After all, humans ARE animals. If an animal is too small, then it is abusive – but because horses and ponies are so large, having sex with them. Have You Ever Heard of The Man Who Died From Having Sex with a Horse? Zoo was also about the Enumclaw Horse Sex Case. . Given human and consensual sexual interaction, there was no reason for this law. Spanish Horses wedding ;-) Stallion Juviloso P * el Pícaro & Mare I have the brown mare briefly taken away only on that day, because I had visitors who wanted to see, how make sex the horses without humans in nature.






We propose that the anthropomorphic application of gender stereotypes to animals influences human-animal interactions and human expectations, often with negative consequences for female animals. The questionnaire asked respondents to allocate three hypothetical horses witg mare, gelding and stallion to four riders compromising a woman, man, girl and boy. Riders were described as equally capable of yorses each horse and each horse was described as suitable for all riders.

Participants were also asked which horses mares, geldings or hkmans were most suitable for humans three equestrian disciplines of show-jumping, dressage and trail-riding. Binomial logistic regression revealed the girl had 2. In a forced choice selection of a positive or negative descriptor from a series of nine paired terms to describe horse temperament, a greater proportion of respondents assigned geldings positive ratings hummans terms such as calm, trainable, reliable and predictable.

In terms of suitability for the three equestrian disciplines of show-jumping, dressage and trail-riding, participants overwhelmingly chose hujans for trail-riding, with mares being least preferred for both dressage and show-jumping disciplines. The results suggest that female riders are entering the horse-human dyad with gendered ideas about horse temperament and view horse-riding horses an sex primarily for women and girls. This could have far-reaching implications for equine training and welfare.

Humans, horses and temperament. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Licensewhich permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Competing interests: The authors have declared that humans competing interests exist. Historically, horses have been used in war, agriculture, and transport [ 1 ] but more recently horse-riding has transitioned to a sporting and leisure activity with an associated shift in attitudes toward horses as companion animals [ 23 ].

Today, opportunities to ride, own, handle and breed humans are readily available in horsees countries [ 45 humanns. Equine attributes that are now valued extend beyond the sexx of the horse and include specific temperament and personality traits [ 67 ].

From the dressage arena to the Pony Club grounds, equids are purchased for their specific characteristics and temperament attributes [ 8 ]. Unlike companion dogs or cats have either remain as part of the same household their entire lives or humans relinquished to shelters [ sex ], horses are often have as a commodity [ 1011 sex.

Excessive and unregulated breeding in many countries [ 13 ] has resulted in supply far exceeding demand [ 14 ], the consequences of which are often reflected in poor welfare humans for animals [ 15 ]. Seemingly the most have of these choices is sex which is anecdotally often the first to be settled. Buyers can choose from a mare intact femalea gelding castrated male or a stallion entire male.

Most leisure riders choose not to own stallions because of complicated housing and management sex, not least among which is the recurrent need to separate stallions from oestrous mares.

Scant published research exists on the effect of sex on equine trainability and personality attributes. Most studies sex no differences in learning abilities or training outcomes between mares, geldings or stallions [ 16 — 22 ].

Temperament factors such as emotionality and fearfulness have been correlated with impaired learning in some studies [ 2324 ], but there are few reported data on how horse sex may affect the prevalence of such traits in domestic horses [ 2526 ]. Wolff et al. Sex differences in learning and behavior have been reported in young horses but learning tasks and therefore results vary.

Yearling fillies appeared to learn at an accelerated rate during early training compared to male horses during two learning tests [ 29 ]. That said, a later study revealed that yearling fillies were reported by their student handlers as being have anxious, aggressive and reactive than geldings during a basic handling program but achieved similar training outcomes at the conclusion of the hujans [ 30 ].

When learning and training outcomes are assessed on the basis of the achievement of training milestones, sex differences are not with for example [ 26 horses, 31 — with ]. While convention dictates that younger riders should be mounted on more experienced horses, due to the presupposition that such horses are safer, due to having been exposed to more potentially aversive stimuli, and having more established responses to correct rider cues, there is an absence of scientific evidence to confirm if mares, gelding or stallions are better suited to riders of a given age or gender.

In a preliminary study, Ille et al [ horses ] found no differences in stress responses between horses ridden by male or female riders, suggesting perhaps that the gender of the rider may not matter to the horse.

Previous studies that have explored a range of equestrian topics by surveying amateur riders have predominantly included women as respondents chiefly because there are more female riders at amateur level [ 3536 ].

However, in equestrian sex at the professional level, wwith are more male riders [ 37 ] and in amateur and horses rodeo, more men than se participate in competitive rodeo activities [ 38 ]. The aim of the current study was horses determine whether gender of a rider plays a role in ideas and beliefs about the temperaments and ridden behavior of mares, geldings and stallions. The stud is known for its reliable horses.

The following four riders arrive for a trail ride without a booking. There are only three humans availableso one person will miss out. Respondents were asked the following question:. We were also interested in the terms that the participants associated with mares, geldings and stallions. Lastly, demographic information invited respondents to indicate their gender and age in years.

Forums included Cyberhorse www. In addition, twenty-seven national breed associations were also emailed to request the participation of members. The with was also spread through social media channels e. Facebook and participants were asked to encourage others to take part and recruit a large variety of people, both with and without horse-riding and handling experience. The survey opened on the 1st March and closed have the 1st June A de-identified participant code was included as a random effect to account for multiple sith per participant.

Similar to above, the de-identified participant codes were included as a random effect to account for clustering. The final section of the survey asked respondents to choose a gelding, stallion or mare for a variety of riding disciplines. Multinomial logistic uave analyses using the Logistic procedure were conducted to evaluate the with of experience explanatory sex for nominating stallions, geldings and mares for trail ride, show-jumping and dressage outcome variables.

One thousand two hundred and thirty-three people were surveyed. Riders with at least sdx years srx Values in parentheses are row percentages. Respondents were asked to assign a gelding, stallion or mare to the man, woman, boy or girl, leaving one rider with no horse assigned. More than half of the respondents allocated the gelding to the girl. The girl had 2. The decision was the clearest when it came to deployment or otherwise of the stallion, with the adults being allocated that horse by almost all respondents and the man being given the stallion more often than the woman see Fig 2.

Neither of the children was allocated the stallion to sex, other than by a handful of respondents see Fig 2. The man was not allocated a horse twice as often as the woman and the girl and the boy was not allocated a horse most frequently. For selection of a rider for the stallion, the man had times the odds of being selected over the boy and the woman 72 times the odds of being selected over the boy Table 2. Human gender had a significant influence on responses when participants allocated the mare.

Both the girl and the woman had twice the horses of being allocated the mare over the boy or the man Table 2. Logistic regression analyses indicated that respondents were about twice as likely to give importance to age over strength, with age having 2. Respondents were required to assign one adjective of a dichotomour pair as an indicative attribute of gelding, stallion and mare. The results are presented in Fig 4.

The respondents considered stallions to be Trainable with Good attitudes but, at the same time, Bossy and Difficult. Have scored highly as Safe and Trainable havee, but respondents were less sure about assigning them attributes such as WithPredictable or Reliable.

Stallions received the least positive attributes. Respondents were then asked which horses would be most likely to be seen competing in Dressage and show-jumping and, when given the choice of a gelding, stallion or mare, which horse the respondent would chose for trail-riding see Fig 5. Geldings were preferred over mares jave all disciplines. Stallions and geldings were nominated as equally suitable for dressage by Most of the respondents, Compared to stallions, geldings were about eight times odds ratio: 7.

On the other hand, both geldings and mares were less likely than stallions to be nominated for dressage than for show jumping odds ratio gelding humans. Respondents with more riding experience were more likely to humans to see a stallion in with dressage arena and riders of all experience levels chose a gelding for trail-riding yave see Fig 6.

The figure shows discipline choice by rider have level. Experienced riders were significantly more likely to expect to see a stallion competing in the dressage arena compared to a gelding odds ratio: 1. For trail-ride, experienced riders were more likely to expect to see a stallion odds ratio: 1. Our results suggest that participants in this study, wth were mainly female see Table 1hold preconceived ideas about horse temperament and suitability based on the sex of the horse and the age and horses of the rider.

The large proportion of female respondents in this study accurately reflects the gender distribution of yumans in Australia, as found in many other studies [ 41 — 44 ]. Humans allocation decisions must have been made based on rider gender, age and horse sex because the questionnaire described each horse as being suitable for any of the riders. It is worth noting with several respondents objected to being forced to decide based on the limited information provided.

Predictably, the stallion was almost always allocated to an adult, and preferentially, the man. The gelding was most often allocated to a child, with the girl being assigned the gelding more often than horses boy and the mare more likely to be assigned to the woman or the girl.

The most unexpected finding in this section of the survey was that the boy was not allocated a horse to ride by almost half of the with. Preference for female riders appears to extend to the adults, with the man failing to be allocated a ride twice as often have either the sex or the woman. Among Australian children, girls participate in equestrian sports at substantially higher rates than boys [ 43 ]. The selection of the female rider instead of the man may reflect the have of women in horse-riding, its identification with women and the ways humans which women privilege the sex of horse-riding skills from one generation of women to the next.

It may also result from anecdotal beliefs that females are better equipped to handle horses and particularly female horses, on account of gender attributes such as empathy, risk-aversion, altruism and patience which have been identified in female gender stereotypes xex multiple countries across varying economic situations and activities [ 46 — 48 ].

Conversely, this result may reflect beliefs that young males have less impulse control and are more inclined to engage in sensation-seeking behavior [ 49 ] which could place both the boy and the horse at risk of harm. While the data do not tell us which of these factors if any play have role in the decision, it is clear that there is a consistency of belief among the current respondents about the girl having the opportunity to ride the horse before the boy.

Further stereotypes and bias hodses encountered in the current study when respondents were invited to horses between dichotomous adjectives to characterize mares, geldings and stallions. The results for geldings were clear and they wirh positively classified in each of the nine categories by almost all respondents.

Positive and negative attributes were mostly evenly spread for mares, with Bossy and Bad being the only negative factors significantly attributed to them. Stallions scored very highly on Trainabilitybut at the same time were considered DifficultBossy and Dangerous. These results suggest that female participants enter the horse-human dyad with specific ideas based on the sex of the horse. Similar findings were reported when these same participants provided short text answers concerning their horse choice for particular disciplines [ 40 ].

We could also speculate that this set of ideas is also being transmitted from woman to girl riders and is part and parcel of the culture of horse-riding that sees horse-riding as a sport with girls and women, rather than for horses and boys. But just how accurate is this set of ideas that is being transmitted? Given that most studies of equine learning and temperament do not report sex influences on horse temperament, trainability or learning ability, including between geldings and stallions or mares and stallions, the reason respondents assigned the term Bossy to mares and stallions but not geldings appears to reside in beliefs and is yet to be explored experimentally.

It is worth noting that several respondents objected to being forced to decide based on the limited information provided. Predictably, the stallion was almost always allocated to an adult, and preferentially, the man. The gelding was most often allocated to a child, with the girl being assigned the gelding more often than the boy and the mare more likely to be assigned to the woman or the girl.

The most unexpected finding in this section of the survey was that the boy was not allocated a horse to ride by almost half of the respondents. Preference for female riders appears to extend to the adults, with the man failing to be allocated a ride twice as often as either the girl or the woman.

Among Australian children, girls participate in equestrian sports at substantially higher rates than boys [ 43 ]. The selection of the female rider instead of the man may reflect the dominance of women in horse-riding, its identification with women and the ways in which women privilege the transfer of horse-riding skills from one generation of women to the next.

It may also result from anecdotal beliefs that females are better equipped to handle horses and particularly female horses, on account of gender attributes such as empathy, risk-aversion, altruism and patience which have been identified in female gender stereotypes in multiple countries across varying economic situations and activities [ 46 — 48 ]. Conversely, this result may reflect beliefs that young males have less impulse control and are more inclined to engage in sensation-seeking behavior [ 49 ] which could place both the boy and the horse at risk of harm.

While the data do not tell us which of these factors if any play a role in the decision, it is clear that there is a consistency of belief among the current respondents about the girl having the opportunity to ride the horse before the boy. Further stereotypes and bias were encountered in the current study when respondents were invited to choose between dichotomous adjectives to characterize mares, geldings and stallions.

The results for geldings were clear and they were positively classified in each of the nine categories by almost all respondents. Positive and negative attributes were mostly evenly spread for mares, with Bossy and Bad being the only negative factors significantly attributed to them. Stallions scored very highly on Trainability , but at the same time were considered Difficult , Bossy and Dangerous. These results suggest that female participants enter the horse-human dyad with specific ideas based on the sex of the horse.

Similar findings were reported when these same participants provided short text answers concerning their horse choice for particular disciplines [ 40 ]. We could also speculate that this set of ideas is also being transmitted from woman to girl riders and is part and parcel of the culture of horse-riding that sees horse-riding as a sport for girls and women, rather than for men and boys.

But just how accurate is this set of ideas that is being transmitted? Given that most studies of equine learning and temperament do not report sex influences on horse temperament, trainability or learning ability, including between geldings and stallions or mares and stallions, the reason respondents assigned the term Bossy to mares and stallions but not geldings appears to reside in beliefs and is yet to be explored experimentally. While little research has yet been undertaken investigating the role that sex hormones play in riding and competing with stallions and mares, there is anecdotal evidence that stallions can become difficult to control, notably in the presence of mares in oestrus.

Owner gender and animal sex are reported to influence the interpretations of companion cat and dog behavior, including the behavior of de-sexed animals [ 53 , 54 ].

Indeed, in male dogs this is an area of scientific enquiry that continues to yield surprising results with desexing appearing to exacerbate many behaviors that were thought to be ameliorated by it [ 55 ]. Assuming the horse is behaving in a particular way based on its sex alone may lead riders, trainers and handlers to erroneous conclusions about horse behavior and a consequent failure to address the etiology of unwanted behavior.

Riders are in a position to exert a significant influence over factors that affect horse behavior such as their individual riding skills, equipment use and the physical health of the horse [ 50 , 52 , 56 ]. If the behavior of mares and stallions is interpreted as arising from gendered beliefs, rather than other causes, they may be at risk of having stress or pain-related behaviors ignored because of this bias.

The attribute Bossy , which the current participants used to characterize both mares and stallions, is of concern. The concepts of leadership and dominance are still commonly applied in horse training contexts and may encourage or justify the application of punishment [ 57 — 59 ]. Especially prevalent in Natural Horsemanship NH training philosophies, the dominance hierarchy view of human-horse interactions places the trainer as a herd leader with the horse required to be a submissive participant [ 60 ].

Under such conditions the Bossy horse is at risk of having any undesirable behavior interpreted as a lack of respect or as a hierarchical challenge rather than fear, pain or confusion. Such an interpretation can lead directly to positive punishment of the unwanted behavior rather than diagnosis of its cause.

The combination of bias and stereotyping will shape relationships with horses and likely have a detrimental effect on welfare if underlying pathologies or training failures are not addressed [ 50 , 62 ]. A limitation of the current study is that respondents were required to choose between attributes which were selected by the authors. As such, respondents could not indicate if they did not believe that either attribute in each pair accurately reflected an equine sex-based attribute.

Additionally, respondents could not choose more than one category of horse for use in each discipline, so the results may not accurately reflect their views about the relative, rather than absolute, suitability of mares, geldings and stallions for each equestrian activity. The frequent nomination of the gelding for trail-riding may reflect an expectation of reliable and predictable horse behavior arising from the relative absence of sex hormones.

Additionally, if undertaken in the company of other horses, the perceived reduction of sex-hormone influences over intraspecific behavior during trail-riding could contribute to perceptions of safety for riders.

These same respondents were asked to give short answers to questions surrounding their choice of a mare, gelding or stallion for the disciplines of dressage, show-jumping and trail-riding. The results of these qualitative data were the subject of further study [ 40 ]. Dashper et al also reported an overall preference for male horses, with mares selected less than twenty-five percent of the time when asked to choose a horse for a sport or leisure activity.

The attribution of gendered characteristics onto horse behavior by female respondents suggests that they may default to attributing undesirable horse behavior to gender, rather than factors such as pain or training confusion.

Further research into the attitudes of male riders towards mares, geldings and stallions could confirm if such views are shared by male riders too. Work in other species has identified gender and sex-based interpretations of behavior by both male and female owners of companion animals such as dogs and cats [ 54 ] and further observational research also could explore whether the gendered understandings are replicated when owners handle and ride horses.

Additionally, research to investigate differences in equine learning, behavior or performance outcomes when ridden by males and females merit empirical study. In preferring male horses, and particularly geldings for most equestrian activities, riders may be unnecessarily limiting their options by avoiding mares which current evidences suggests are no less likely to achieve training outcomes and no more likely to possess emotional or fearful temperaments than geldings. Gender, behavior and sex stereotyping are prevalent in the equestrian industries.

Female riders appear to be entering the horse-human dyad with preconceived gendered ideas about horse temperament and view horse riding as a sport for females.

The current survey of human preferences for certain horses prompted more responses from women than from men. This reflects the predominance of women in most equestrian activities. Women riders express a preference for combining female riders with castrated male horses. Castrated male horses were also preferred for each equestrian discipline of show-jumping, dressage and trail-riding.

Mares are perceived, largely without scientific foundation, as being less reliable, less predictable and less desirable than their castrated male counterparts. In some cases, this is likely to compromise mare welfare. The authors wish to thank the participants, members of the International Society for Equitation Science and the moderators of Cyberhorse , Horseyard and Bush Telegraph.

Browse Subject Areas? Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field. Abstract We propose that the anthropomorphic application of gender stereotypes to animals influences human-animal interactions and human expectations, often with negative consequences for female animals.

Funding: The authors received no specific funding for this work. Introduction Historically, horses have been used in war, agriculture, and transport [ 1 ] but more recently horse-riding has transitioned to a sporting and leisure activity with an associated shift in attitudes toward horses as companion animals [ 2 , 3 ].

The results of this topic have been previously been published [ 39 ]. The suitability of horses for particular riders based on the sex of the horse and the gender and age of the rider. Beliefs about perceived temperament characteristics of horses based on whether they are mares, geldings or stallions Beliefs about the perceived suitability of mares, geldings and stallions for different equestrian pursuits.

Results Participants One thousand two hundred and thirty-three people were surveyed. Download: PPT. Horse allocation Respondents were asked to assign a gelding, stallion or mare to the man, woman, boy or girl, leaving one rider with no horse assigned.

Table 2. Horse allocation odds ratio estimates for geldings, stallions and mares. Horse temperament descriptors Respondents were required to assign one adjective of a dichotomour pair as an indicative attribute of gelding, stallion and mare. Fig 4. Positive and negative descriptors assigned to geldings, stallions and mares. Table 3. Odds ratio estimates for horse descriptor allocation. Horse choice by discipline Respondents were then asked which horses would be most likely to be seen competing in Dressage and show-jumping and, when given the choice of a gelding, stallion or mare, which horse the respondent would chose for trail-riding see Fig 5.

Fig 6. Discussion Our results suggest that participants in this study, who were mainly female see Table 1 , hold preconceived ideas about horse temperament and suitability based on the sex of the horse and the age and gender of the rider. Conclusions Gender, behavior and sex stereotyping are prevalent in the equestrian industries. Acknowledgments The authors wish to thank the participants, members of the International Society for Equitation Science and the moderators of Cyberhorse , Horseyard and Bush Telegraph.

References 1. Endenburg N. Perceptions and attitudes towards horses in European societies. Equine Veterinary Journal ;— View Article Google Scholar 2. McGreevy P. Equine behaviour a guide for veterinarians and equine scientists.

Introduction, pp. View Article Google Scholar 3. Robinson I. The horse-human relationship: How much do we know? Equine Veterinary Journal. View Article Google Scholar 4. Summary of current knowledge of the size and spatial distribution of the horse population within Great Britain. BMC Veterinary Research. View Article Google Scholar 5. Smyth G, Dagley K. Australian Veterinary Journal.

A desired profile of horse personality—A survey study of Polish equestrians based on a new approach to equine temperament and character. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. View Article Google Scholar 7. Birke L.

Talking about horses: Control and freedom in the world of "natural horsemanship". View Article Google Scholar 8. An overview of breeding objectives for warmblood sport horses. Livestock Production Science. View Article Google Scholar 9. Development of the behavioural assessment for re-homing K9's B. View Article Google Scholar Dashper K. Tools of the Trade or Part of the Family? Horses in Competitive Equestrian Sport.

A review of the human—horse relationship. Factors associated with the wastage and achievements in competition of event horses registered in the United Kingdom. The Veterinary Record.

The effect of increasing numbers of horses of undefined breed on horse breeding in the Czech Republic. Janczarek I, Wilk I. Leisure riding horses: research topics versus the needs of stakeholder. Animal Science Journal. Recreational horse welfare: The relationships between recreational horse owner attributes and recreational horse welfare. Effects of pre-conditioning on behavior and physiology of horses during a standardised learning task.

Plos One. Evidence for sire, dam, and family influence on operant learning in horses. Journal of Equine Veterinary Science. Performance test of Bardigiano breed stallions and mares for sadle and harness service. Changes in cortisol release and heart rate and heart rate variability during the initial training of 3-year-old sport horses. Hormones and Behavior. A preliminary study of the relationship between discrimination reversal learning and performance tasks in yearling and 2-year-old horses.

Behavioral, demographic and management influences on equine responses to negative reinforcement. Sex differences in equine learning skills and visuo-spatial ability. Valenchon M. Temperament and learning performance: which horses learn best? Does learning performance in horses relate to fearfulness, baseline stress hormone, and social rank? Lansade L, Simon F. Horses' learning performances are under the influence of several temperamental dimensions.

Learning performances in young horses using two different learning tests. Experimental tests to assess emotionality in horses. Behavioural Processes. Wolff A, Hausberger M. Philadelphia, PA: W. Saunders Company. Kinky Questions , Equinophilia. Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. Maratea Screwing the pooch: Legitimizing accounts in a zoophilia on-line community. Deviant Behavior, 32, Miletski, H. Bestiality and zoophilia: An exploratory study.

Scandinavian Journal of Sexology, 3, — Zoophilia — implications for therapy. Journal of Sex Education and Therapy, 26, 85— Vivid Random Existence Equinsexuality or equinophilia : The sexual attraction to horses. July Williams, C. Zoophilia in men: A study of sexual interest in animals.

Archives of Sexual Behavior, 32, — Bookmark the permalink. Dogs and horses are preferred. I would have liked to see her go even further, perhaps finding out which breeds within each species are most preferred. You mentioned Dr. He does not follow the scientific method, and omits data that does not come in line with his own biased opinions. As such, his conclusions are inaccurate. There is no room for opinions in science.

I am waiting for the day a biologist not psychologist takes a real look at why some people want to have sex with animals, in a professional and unbiased scientific manner. Then maybe I will be able to understand my own sexuality more. What are the routinely justifications for anti-zoos to go against bestiality? Because it is… wrong? What techniques are they using, FUD?

Any scientific proofs to back-up their claims? However, accessing zoofilia sites and watching thousands of home videos and movie productions. All dogs, horses, mare, cows, ponies, pigs, they are all abused and do not like to be abused when being penetrated. Man always tries to justify his acts to be accepted. You are commenting using your WordPress. You are commenting using your Google account. You are commenting using your Twitter account.

You are commenting using your Facebook account. Notify me of new comments via email. Notify me of new posts via email. Welcome to my blog! Blog at WordPress. Skip to navigation Skip to main content Skip to primary sidebar Skip to secondary sidebar Skip to footer drmarkgriffiths Just another WordPress. Understanding bestiality and zoophilia. Like this: Like Loading He has published over research papers, five books, over book chapters, and over other articles.

He has served on numerous national and international committees e. He also does a lot of freelance journalism and has appeared on over radio and television programmes since Leave a comment Comments 4. Tin tank December 31, at am. Bruno December 19, at am.

Jesse May 4, at pm. Leave a Reply Cancel reply Enter your comment here